Skip to Content

Who has greater IQ?

Intelligence Quotient (IQ) has long been used as a measure of cognitive ability and capacity for learning. Researchers have conducted numerous studies comparing IQ scores across different groups and demographics. In this article, we will explore research on IQ disparities between different genders, ethnicities, and socioeconomic groups.

Do men or women have higher IQ scores on average?

When looking at IQ disparities between genders, multiple analyses have found that men tend to score higher on IQ tests than women. However, the differences are modest. In a review of over 30 studies, researchers found that men scored approximately 3-5 IQ points higher than women on average (1).

One potential explanation for this disparity is differences in brain structure and composition between men and women. Studies show that men tend to have larger brain volumes in areas associated with cognition. However, women tend to have greater connectivity between the hemispheres of the brain, which may give advantages in integrative thinking (2).

Cultural factors and stereotypes may also play a role in IQ testing disparities between genders. Some experts argue that IQ tests may be inadvertently biased in ways that favor males. For example, having more questions related to visual-spatial skills, which tend to be stronger in men (3).

Overall, while men score slightly higher on average, there is substantial overlap in the IQ distributions of both genders. When looking at the highest levels of cognitive ability, there are equal numbers of men and women. This suggests differences in average IQ scores are heavily influenced by sociocultural factors rather than inherent cognitive differences between genders (4).

Conclusion

Research shows a very modest IQ advantage for males over females. However, this gap is narrowing over time and is likely explained by cultural biases rather than biological differences.

Which ethnic groups have the highest IQ scores?

Studies exploring IQ disparities between ethnic groups are controversial due to the ugly history of racism masquerading as science. Nonetheless, modern research that controls for socioeconomic status finds persistent IQ gaps between certain ethnic populations.

In the US, Asian Americans as a group consistently score higher on IQ tests than caucasians, who in turn score higher than Hispanic and African American populations (5). However, there are significant variances within broad ethnic groupings. For example, some Asian ethnicities, such as Vietnamese Americans, score lower than caucasian Americans (6).

As with gender, these group differences are modest, with substantial overlap between distributions. Moreover, the gaps appear strongly linked to socioeconomic factors. When controlling for parental income, education, and occupational status, the differences in IQ scores between ethnicities are dramatically reduced (7).

This suggests much of the disparity can be attributed to environmental factors, rather than inherent differences in cognitive ability between ethnic populations. Discrimination and lack of access to resources likely contribute to artificially suppressing IQ scores among disadvantaged minority groups.

Conclusion

Current data shows modest IQ differences between broad ethnic groupings. However, these gaps largely disappear when controlling for socioeconomic status, indicating environmental factors play a key role.

Does IQ correlate with socioeconomic status?

Many studies have found a robust correlation between IQ and socioeconomic status (SES). People who are higher in social class tend to score better on IQ tests. This link persists even when controlling for ethnicity (8).

For example, an analysis of over 45,000 children found that children of parents with professional occupations scored on average 16.5 IQ points higher than children of unskilled laborers (9). The gap was present across ethnic groups.

There are several explanations thought to contribute to this disparity:

  • Access to resources – Higher SES families can provide enriching learning materials and experiences that may boost IQ.
  • Genetics – Parental IQ correlates with child IQ, so these differences may partially reflect inherited intelligence.
  • Family stress – Poverty can create stressful home environments that may impair cognitive development.

Disentangling these factors remains an active area of research. But the bulk of evidence indicates environmental influences play a substantial role in the IQ-SES relationship (10).

Conclusion

Higher socioeconomic status is strongly associated with higher IQ scores. This relationship persists across ethnic groups, suggesting environmental factors are a significant contributor.

How much does education influence IQ?

Education also appears to have a significant influence on IQ testing performance. One study followed over 17,000 individuals and assessed their IQ at ages 10 and 33 (11). Results showed that after adjusting for parental SES, years of education significantly predicted IQ at age 33 but not at age 10.

Each additional year of education was associated with a 1.6 point increase in IQ on average. Furthermore, the boost in IQ remained even when accounting for occupation, income, and social status in adulthood. This indicates education itself imparted benefits beyond subsequent socioeconomic gains.

Researchers propose that schooling aids the development of skills and knowledge critical for success on IQ tests. These include abilities related to language, reasoning, problem-solving, and working memory (12). The intellectual stimulation of education appears to produce cognitive gains that are retained and measurable later in life.

Conclusion

Access to education predicts higher IQ in adulthood, even when accounting for other socioeconomic factors. Schooling aids intellectual development in skills assessed by IQ tests.

Does nutrition influence intelligence?

Nutrition also plays an important role in cognitive development and IQ outcomes. Multiple studies on malnutrition have found that undernutrition, especially in early childhood, can adversely impact cognitive functioning.

For example, one analysis examining IQ in adults who experienced severe malnutrition as children found their IQ was lower by an average of 5 points compared to siblings who did not experience undernutrition (13). Other research has linked vitamin and mineral deficiencies (e.g. iron, iodine, folate) during pregnancy and early childhood to lasting reductions in IQ (14).

Beyond malnutrition, higher socioeconomic status is associated with better nutrition more generally. Access to adequate nutrition supports healthy brain development and higher IQ. This may be another pathway in which poverty can disadvantage cognitive performance.

Conclusion

Poor nutrition, especially in early childhood, is linked to lasting reductions in IQ scores. Along with education and resources, nutrition is another socioeconomic factor that contributes to IQ disparities.

What is the relationship between IQ and genetics?

Twin studies suggest genetics do play a meaningful role in determining IQ. Identical twins raised apart were found to correlate at .72 for IQ compared to .42 for fraternal twins. This indicates a substantial heritable component (15). However, environment still accounts for over a quarter of variance in IQ.

Currently, scientists estimate that 40-80% of variance in IQ scores across a population can be attributed to genetic differences (16). However, this does not mean IQ is fixed or impervious to environmental influences at the individual level.

Gene-environment interactions are also important – access to resources may allow high genetic potential to flourish, while adversity can stunt development. The high heritability of IQ at the population level does not preclude the importance of environmental factors in shaping individual cognitive abilities.

Conclusion

IQ is influenced by both genetic and environmental factors. Genes set boundaries but environment determines where individuals fall within those boundaries. Both nature and nurture shape cognitive ability.

Can IQ scores be increased in adulthood?

For many years it was believed that IQ remained largely stable across the lifespan after early childhood. However, recent research has indicated that IQ scores can continue to change through adolescence and even into adulthood.

Multiple longitudinal studies tracking individuals over time indicate that on average, IQ scores increase as people age. For example, one analysis found that IQ increased by 1.7 points per decade on average between ages 33 and 53 (17).

It is theorized that accumulated knowledge and experience over time contribute to improved test performance. Though people’s relative rank order often remains stable, intellectual capacity continues developing throughout life.

Targeted cognitive training may also boost IQ scores. A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials found programs teaching problem-solving skills increased IQ by an average of 3-5 points (18). Gains were larger for programs targeted at children versus adults.

Conclusion

Intelligence test performance can improve through at least middle adulthood due to accumulated knowledge and cognitive training effects. IQ is not fixed in early childhood.

What are the key takeaways?

In summary, research on group differences in IQ leads to the following conclusions:

  • Modest IQ gaps exist between genders and ethnicities, but substantial overlap occurs between distributions.
  • Socioeconomic factors including education, resources, and nutrition strongly predict IQ outcomes.
  • IQ is shaped by both genes and environment – neither is deterministic on its own.
  • Cognitive ability continues developing through adulthood and is not fixed from childhood.

Though average IQ scores may differ across demographic groups, individuals exist across the entire spectrum within each group. With supportive conditions, people remain capable of intellectual growth throughout life.

Conclusion

While group differences exist, IQ is largely influenced by socioeconomic factors and continues developing through adulthood. Neither genes nor environment determine cognitive destiny.